Page last updated: Wednesday, November 19, 2008
Is there a J-shaped relationship between alcohol consumption and cognitive function with ageing?
by
Creina S. Stockley, Health and Regulatory Information Manager, The Australian Wine Research Institute
Introduction

There is a clear J-shaped relationship between alcohol consumption and the risk of cardiovascular disease which has been extended to a reduced risk of certain cancers (Boffetta and Garfinkel 1990, Marmot and Brunner 1991, Thun et al. 1997). Over the last five years, evidence has accumulated which suggests that this J-shaped relationship could also be extended to a reduced risk of cognitive dysfunction. Cognitive function is defined as the intellectual or mental processes by which knowledge is acquired, including perception, reasoning, acts of creativity, problem-solving and possible intuition.

Cognitive dysfunction or impairment is associated with increased disability and an increased need for institutionalised care, especially in an ageing population. While cardiovascular disease accounted for 40% of all Australian deaths in 2000 and cancer accounted for 25-30%, 6% of the Australian population aged 65 years and over were diagnosed with dementia. Dementia is a form of cognitive dysfunction whereby an individual loses the ability to think, remember and reason due to physical changes in the brain. Given the anticipated ageing of the Australian population over the next 50 years, for example, the median age of Australia’s population is projected to increase from 35.4 years in 2000 to 46.5 years in 2050 (ABS 2003), research on risk factors for these diseases is a national priority.

Relationship between alcohol and cognitive function

Prior to a study by Zuccala et al. (2001), there was conflicting evidence on the relationship between alcohol consumption and cognitive function (Cervilla et al., 2000; Dent et al., 1997; Dufouil et al., 1997; Elias et al., 1999; Harwood et al., 1999; Hendrie et al., 1996; Leibovici et al., 1999; Teri et al., 1990). Zuccala et al. (2001) analyzed the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment in 15,807 hospitalized older patients who were enrolled in an Italian multicentre pharmacoepidemiology survey. The probability of cognitive impairment was reduced among male patients who reported an average daily alcohol consumption of 1 L or less of wine, as compared with abstainers, but the probability increased among heavier drinkers. Among women, only the lightest-drinking category (<0.5 L) showed a decreased probability of cognitive dysfunction when compared with abstainers, whereas heavier drinking was associated with an increased probability of cognitive impairment. The prevalence of alcohol abuse was similar among participants with cognitive impairment (0.9%) and those with normal cognitive functioning (1%). The results of this study indicated that moderate alcohol consumption, that is, <40 g per day for women and <80 g for men, is associated with reduced probability of cognitive impairment as compared with abstinence, after adjusting for potential confounders. This nonlinear association persisted when cerebrovascular and Alzheimer’s disease were considered separately. Such a nonlinear association might explain the conflicting results of previous studies regarding the relationship between alcohol consumption and cognitive functioning.

The observed gender difference in amount of alcohol consumption necessary for improved cognitive function, confirms that observed by Elias et al. in 1999 who showed that ‘superior’ cognitive performance was found with in the range of four to eight drinks per day for men but only two to four drinks per day for women, compared to abstainers.

Subsequent studies have also independently assessed the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive function and have affirmed the observations of Zuccala et al. (2001) but have also provided more detailed data (Ganguli, et al. 2005, Stampfer et al. 2005, McDougall et al. 2006, Reid et al. 2006, Wright et al. 2006). For example, Reid et al. (2006) in a study of 760 US men aged 65 years or older showed that current light to moderate alcohol consumption considered as up to seven drinks per week, compared to abstinence, had better cognitive function. In particular, processing speed, which is the ability to perform tasks requiring rapid visual scanning and mental processing of information, was better even after adjusting for potential confounders such as education and occupation. In addition, the study assessed the effects of cumulative lifetime alcohol consumption on cognitive function and showed that the number of years of light to moderate alcohol consumption was strongly associated with better cognitive function. Results from a small survey study by McDougall et al. (2006) also suggested that men aged 65 years or older who drank moderately had significantly less depression, higher self-reported general health and higher cognitive function, flexibility and verbal memory.

Several studies had shown that the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive function is stronger for women than for men, which may simply reflect a gender difference in cognitive function or perhaps a misclassification of moderate alcohol consumers. Such a gender difference was not, however, observed by in a longitudinal study of 1624 Japanese American men and women aged over 65 years (Bond et al. 2005). Other studies which assessed women specifically, such as the US Nurses’ Health Study, suggested that for women aged 71 to 80 years, up to 15g alcohol per day did not impair cognitive function and actually improved it compared to abstinence (Stampfer et al. 2005); the women also had a decreased risk of cognitive impairment of approximately 20%. No significant differences were observed in cognitive performance or risk between beer and wine consumers. Furthermore, a study of women aged 65-80 years, showed that women consuming any alcohol performed better on tests of verbal knowledge, fluency and memory, and figural memory, attention and working memory and motor speed compared to abstainers (P<0.05) (Espeland et al. 2006). After covariate adjustment, mean scores were higher among women reporting ≥1 drink/day by 5.7% for verbal knowledge (p<0.001) and by 5.7% for phonemic fluency (p=0.004), compared to abstainers.

Potential mechanisms of action

The beneficial effects of alcohol on the risk of cardiovascular and cerebrovascular diseases, such as heart attacks and strokes, have been partly attributed to changes in lipid and haemostatic or blood flow factors (Rimm et al., 1999). These changes include alcohol-induced increases in the concentration of high density lipoprotein-cholesterol and the thrombolyic proteins tissue type plasminogen activator activity and tissue type plasminogen activator antigen, and alcohol-induced reductions in fibrinogen, and clotting cofactors factor VII and von Willebrand factor. These changes are also associated with atherosclerosis which is the accumulation of atheromatous plaques containing cholesterol and lipids on the innermost layer of the walls of large and medium-sized arteries. As atherosclerosis has been associated with both Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia, it had been suggested that any beneficial effect of alcohol on atherosclerosis could be expected to benefit these major subtypes of dementia by preserving brain vasculature, consequently resulting in better cognitive function. Wright et al. (2006), however, showed that the appearance of plaque on the carotid artery which carries blood to the brain was not associated with alcohol consumption and alcohol-associated improvements in cognitive function. This suggests then that alcohol may impact cognition through a separate vascular or degenerative pathway. Among older persons without cerebrovascular disease, those who moderately consume alcohol have been shown to have fewer white-matter abnormalities and infarcts on magnetic resonance imaging than abstainers (Mukamal et al. 2001), where pronounced reductions in the risk of both vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease have been shown among persons consuming one to six standard drinks per week (Mukamal et al. 2003).

Indeed, there is also evidence which suggests that a light to moderate amount of alcohol may stimulate the release of acetylcholine in the hippocampus leading to improved cognitive function such that a light amount of alcohol in normal subjects appears to improve memory for events experienced before consumption where the impairment of memory performance by chronic and heavy alcohol consumption parallels the reduction of acetylcholine neurotransmission (Fadda and Rossetti 1998).

Conclusions

The protective effect of light to moderate alcohol consumption against cognitive dysfunction, including dementia (Simons et al. 2006), has been consistently observed over the past five years. Thus, while excessive alcohol consumption should be avoided, it would appear safe and reasonable to recommend the continuation of light to moderate alcohol consumption in those already imbibing.

References

References

Australian Bureau of Statistics, Estimated Resident Population; Population Projections, Australia, 2002-2101, (ABS cat. no. 3222.0), 2003; United Nations Population Division, World Population Prospects: The 2002 Revision, <http://www.unpopulation.org>, accessed 3 September 2003.

Boffetta, P., Garfinkel, L. (1990) Alcohol drinking and mortality among men enrolled in an American Cancer Society prospective study. Epidemiology 1: 342–8.

Bond, G.E., Burr, R.L., McCurry, S.M., Rice, M.M., Borenstein, A.R., Larson, E.B. (2005) Alcohol and cognitive performance: a longitudinal study of older Japanese Americans. The Kame Project. Int Psychogeriatr. 17(4):653-68.

Cervilla, J.A., Prince, M., Mann, A. (2000) Smoking, drinking, and incident cognitive impairment: a cohort community based study included in the Gospel Oak project. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 68:622–6.

Dent, O.F., Sulway, M.R., Broe, G.A., Creasey, H., Kos, S.C., Jorm, A.F., Tennant, C., Fairley, M.J. (1997) Alcohol consumption and cognitive performance in a random sample of Australian soldiers who served in the second world war. Br Med J 314:1655-7.

Dufouil, C., Ducimetiere, P., Alperovitch, A (1997) Sex differences in the association between alcohol consumption and cognitive performance. Am J Epidemiol 146:405–12.

Elias, P.K., Elias, M.F., D’Agostino, R.B. Silbershatz, H. Wolf, P.A. (1999) Alcohol consumption and cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study. Am J Epidemiol 150:580–9.

Espeland, M.A., Coker, L.H., Wallace, R., Rapp, S.R., Resnick, S.M., Limacher, M., Powell, L.H., Messina, C.R. (2006) Association between Alcohol Intake and Domain-Specific Cognitive Function in Older Women. Neuroepidemiology 27(1):1-12.

Fadda, F., Rossetti, Z.L. (1998) Chronic ethanol consumption: from neuroadaptation to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol. 56(4):385-431.

Harwood, D.G., Barker, W.W., Loewenstein, D.A., Ownby, R.L., St. George-Hyslop, P., Mullan, M., Duara, R. (1999) A cross-ethnic analysis of risk factors for AD in white Hispanics and white non-Hispanics. Neurology 52:551–6.

Hendrie, H.C., Gao, S., Hall, K.S., Hui, S.L., Unverzagt, F.W. (1996) The relationship between alcohol consumption, cognitive performance, and daily functioning in an urban sample of older black Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:1158–65.

Ganguli, M., Vander Bilt, J., Saxton, J.A., Shen, C., Dodge, H.H. (2005) Alcohol consumption and cognitive function in late life: A longitudinal community study Neurology 65:1210-17.

Leibovici, D., Ritchie, K., Ledesert, B., Touchon, J (1999) The effects of wine and tobacco consumption on cognitive performance in the elderly: a longitudinal study of relative risk. Int J Epidemiol 28:77–81.

Marmot, M., Brunner, E. (1991) Alcohol and cardiovascular disease: the status of the U shaped curve. Br Med J 303:565–8.

McDougall, G.J. Jr, Becker, H., Areheart, K.L. (2006) Older males, cognitive function, and alcohol consumption. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 27(4):33-53.

Mukamal, K.J., Longstreth, W.T. Jr, Mittleman, M.A., Crum, R.M., Siscovick, D.S. (2001) Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Stroke 32:1939-46.

Mukamal, K.J., Kuller, L.H., Fitzpatrick, A.L., Longstreth, W.T. Jr, Mittleman, M.A., Siscovick, D.S. (2003) Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults. JAMA 289:1405-13.

Reid, M.C., Van Ness, P.H., Hawkins, K.A., Towle, V., Concato, J., Guo, Z. (2006) Light to moderate alcohol consumption is associated with better cognitive function among older male veterans receiving primary care. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 19(2):98-105.

Rimm, E.B., Williams, P., Fosher, K., Criqui, M., Stampfer, M.J. (1999) Moderate alcohol intake and lower risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of effects on lipids and haemostatic factors. Br Med J 319:1523–8.

Simons, L.A., Simons, J., McCallum, J., Friedlander, Y. (2006) Lifestyle factors and risk of dementia: Dubbo Study of the elderly. Med J Aust. 184(2):68-70.

Stampfer, M.J., Kang, J.H., Chen, J., Cherry, R., Grodstein, F. (2005) Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive function in women. N Engl J Med. 352(3):245-53.

Teri, L., Hughes, J.P., Larson, E.B. (1990) Cognitive deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease: behavioural and health factors. J Gerontol 42:3–10.

Thun, M.J., Peto, R., Lopez, A.D., Monaco, J.H., Henley, S.J., Heath, C.W. Jr, Doll, R (1997) Alcohol consumption and mortality among middle-aged and elderly U.S. adults. N Engl J Med 337:1705–14.

Wright, C.B., Elkind, M.S.V., Rundek, T., Boden-Albala, B., Paik, M.C., Sacco, R.L. (2006) Alcohol Intake, Carotid Plaque, and Cognition: The Northern Manhattan Study Stroke 37:1160-4.

Zuccala, G., Onder, G., Pedone, C., Cesari, M., Landi, F., Bernabei, R., Cocchi, A. (2001) Gruppo Italiano di Farmacoepidemiologia nell'Anziano Investigators. Dose-related impact of alcohol consumption on cognitive function in advanced age: results of a multicenter survey. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 25(12):1743-8.

no website link
All text and images © 2003 Alcohol In Moderation.